In a very pretty extension of the same idea, Roger Tootell, in Russel De Valois's laboratory at Berkeley, had an animal look with one eye at a large pattern of concentric circles and rays, shown in the top image of the figure to the left. In this experiment by Roger Tootell, the target-shaped stimulus with radial lines was centered on an anesthetized macaque monkey's right visual field for 45 minutes after injection with radioactive 2- deoxyglucose. One eye was held closed. The lower picture shows the labeling in the striate cortex of the left hemisphere. This autoradiograph shows a section parallel to the surface; the cortex was flattened and frozen before sectioning. The roughly vertical lines of label represent the (semi)circular stimulus lines; the horizontal lines of label represent the radial lines in the right visual field. The hatching within each line of label is caused by only one eye having been stimulated and represents ocular- dominance columns. The resulting pattern on the cortex contains the circles and rays, distorted just as expected by the variations in magnification (the distance on the cortex corresponding to 1 degree of visual field), a phenomenon related to the change in visual acuity between the fovea and periphery of the eye. Over and above that, each circle or ray is broken up by the fine ocular- dominance stripes. Stimulating both eyes would have resulted in continuous bands. Seldom can we illustrate so many separate facts so neatly, all in a single experiment. Cats, several kinds of monkeys, chimpanzees, and man all possess ocular- dominance columns. The columns are absent in rodents and tree shrews; and although hints of their presence can be detected physiologically in the squirrel monkey, a new world monkey, present anatomical methods do not reveal the columns. At present we don't know what purpose this highly patterned segregation of eye influence serves, but one guess is that it has something to do with stereopsis (see Chapter 7). Subdivisions of the cortex by specialization in cell function have been found in many regions besides the striate cortex. They were first seen in the somato-sensory cortex by Vernon Mountcastle in the mid- 1950s, in what was surely the most important set of observations on cortex since localization of function was first discovered. The somatosensory is to touch, pressure, and joint position what the striate cortex is to vision. Mountcastle showed that this cortex is similarly subdivided vertically into regions in which cells are sensitive to touch and regions in which cells respond to bending of joints or applying deep pressure to a limb. Like ocular-dominance columns, the regions are about half a millimeter across, but whether they form stripes, a checkerboard, or an ocean- and-islands pattern is still not clear. The term column was coined by Mountcastle, so one can probably assume that he had a pillarlike structure in mind. We now know that the word slab would be more suitable for the visual cortex. Terminology is hard to change, however, and it seems best to stick to the well-known term, despite its shortcomings. Today we speak of columnar subdivisions when some cell attribute remains constant from surface to white matter and varies in records taken parallel to the surface. For reasons that will become clear in the next chapter, we usually restrict the concept to exclude the topographic representation, that is, position of receptive fields on the retina or position on the body.